Emerging Concepts of Posture and Alignment
Donna Krasnow, M.S., Rita Monasterio, M.S., and Steven J. Chatfield, Ph.D.
Medical Problems of Performing Artists, March 2001.
Dance educators have been posing questions and theories
about the alignment of the body for centuries. In The
Life and Works of John Weaver, Ralph [50] includes lectures by
Weaver, written in 1721, in which he describes good posture
for the dancer. Blasis5 first published An Elementary Treatise
Upon the Theory and Practice of the Art of Dancing in 1820, in
which he expounds in detail about the correct placement of
the segments of the dancer’s body. By the 1900s these concepts
of alignment were becoming extended, and detailed discussions
of the importance of teaching proper alignment
began to appear in the literature. [32,34]
The simplest and perhaps most common approach to this
issue is one familiar to all dance educators, viewing the body
in erect quiet stance. The dancer is observed from the side,
and an imaginary “plumb line” is dropped from the top of
the head down to the feet to assess how closely the centers of
the various body segments and joints (head, shoulders, rib
cage, hips, knees, ankles) approximate this line. The dancer
is also observed from the front and back to check the bilateral
symmetry of the body. What information or insight does
this process of assessing alignment in stance yield? And how
does this relate to what occurs once the body is in motion?
Does the vertical placement of the body in quiet stance prepare
it for the dynamic moment-by-moment adjustments
needed to maintain equilibrium as the body moves in space,
continually challenged by disturbances to balance? And how
much of a dancer’s balance and alignment is volitional effort,
versus unconscious neural activation of muscles? If it is the
latter, can these postural neuromuscular responses be
enhanced through training?
It is easy to assume that static or quiet stance is a posture
that is motionless and therefore does not require moment by
moment muscular adjustments. However, Hellebrandt and
colleagues25 demonstrated that vertical, standing posture is
not static, but rather it is movement occurring on a stationary
base, referred to as postural sway. All postures of the
human body, whether in quiet stance or moving through
space, require ongoing muscular effort and adaptation. The
somatic practices [1,2,19,24,56,57] explore the idea that alignment is
dynamic and controlled at a neural level. The studies in the field of motor learning and motor control examine posture
as an ongoing process of neuromuscular responses to disturbances,
or perturbations, to balance. [3,6,7,10,13,14,17,26,45,54,63]
For the purposes of this writing, static vertical alignment
is defined as a view of skeletal placement along a plumb line,
viewed from the side, with the body segments stacked on the
line of gravity in a non-locomotion stance, and with the
weight evenly distributed between the feet. This can be considered
a neutral “home base” for the body. Although it is
not truly “static,” this terminology is used to differentiate this
idea of alignment from that of dynamic alignment. Dynamic
alignment is defined as an ongoing process of neuromuscular
postural responses occurring at an unconscious level, and
can refer to the body in stance or in motion, in a variety of
conditions. The purpose of this article is twofold: (1) to
review, examine, and combine these various perspectives on
posture and alignment; and (2) to propose training and
research approaches suggested by emerging concepts.
STATIC VERTICAL ALIGNMENT
In the 1966 edition of Kinesiology: The Scientific Basis of
Human Motion, Wells60 defends the practice of assessing the
alignment of subjects in the erect standing posture. She states
that while this position is of little importance in and of itself,
it is important as “the point of departure for the many postural
patterns assumed by the individual, both at rest and in
motion . . . its importance is in direct proportion to the extent
to which it represents the individual’s habitual carriage [emphasis
hers]” (p. 391). Past and recent studies have used static vertical
stance as a way of assessing alignment in dancers.
Woodhull-McNeal and colleagues [62] analyzed the alignment
of thirteen female college dancers in four common
dance positions (parallel first, and turned out first, third, and
fifth) by photographing the dancers from the side, and then
measuring distances between bony landmarks, including
ankle, knee, hip joint, pelvis, shoulder, and ear. The measure
of best alignment was represented by values coming closest to
being in a straight line. Results of this study indicated that
alignment in turned out first was significantly closer to a
straight line than the other three dance positions. Additionally,
anterior pelvic tilt was significantly greater in fifth position
than the other three positions. This suggests that alignment
is variable by condition.
Fairweather and Sidaway [18] conducted two studies observing
alignment in quiet stance. The first study used the Wickens-
Kiphuth method for alignment analysis. This system involves attaching rods to particular spinal landmarks, taking
photos of the subject from the side, and then drawing connecting
lines between dots on the photos. These lines are
then used to calculate various misalignments, assessed in
quiet stance. Their second study used a dynamic motion
analysis system, Peak Performance, but the measurements
were still taken only in quiet stance.
The first study involved 15 physically active high school
males with histories of low back pain. The second involved
physically active males and females, aged 18–23 years. Both
studies included an experimental group that participated in
ideokinetic imagery, an experimental group that did abdominal
strength exercises, and flexibility work for the low back and
for the hip flexors, and a control group. The second experiment
also had a relaxation group, to determine whether
ideokinesis or simply relaxation was causing the beneficial
effects. In both studies, pre- and post-testing measured changes
in sagittal curvatures of kyphosis and lordosis. Both experiments
resulted in significant differences in the ideokinetic
imagery group only, for male subjects, who showed reduced
lordotic curvature. In subjective interviews, the ideokinetic
imagery group also reported long-term reduction of low back
pain. This study suggests that improving alignment patterns
should include a component of neural retraining, and that
ideokinesis is an effective method of achieving this retraining.
It is a known procedure in dance training settings to do
assessments of alignment by observing the subject in static
stance from the side, often in front of a grid or using a plumb
bob. [20,48] The assessment procedures differ in the exact location
of the line of gravity, such as whether the line should fall
anterior or posterior to the ear, or whether it should fall
directly through or anterior to the lateral malleolus. In spite
of these differences, generally the plumb line passes through
or near the following bony landmarks: the mastoid process,
the center of the shoulder joint, the greater trochanter, the
center of the knee joint, and the lateral malleolus.
Researchers have also used this method of examining alignment
in dancers. Kerr and colleagues31 assessed postural alignment
by developing a prototype and a rating system based on
skeletal placement of five body areas in quiet stance, and then
having dance experts rate the dancers standing in front of a
grid. The five body areas targeted were: (1) head, neck, shoulders,
and cervical spine; (2) rib cage and thoracic spine; (3)
lumbar spine, pelvis, and hip joints; (4) knees, lower legs,
ankles, and feet; and (5) relationship of the center of weight
to the base of support. The rating system was developed using
the assessment procedures outlined by Fitt20 and Plastino48
and descriptions of alignment in the literature. [18,30,34,56,60,62]
What all of these perspectives share is a basic understanding
that (1) alignment can be measured or assessed by examining
the placement of bony landmarks during quiet stance,
and (2) the dancer whose posture comes closer to a vertically
stacked line is “better,” meaning more efficient, less injuryprone,
and more aesthetically pleasing. While these two ideas
are fundamentally still prevalent in much of the current
dance training, there is still the question as to whether this
can be achieved through conscious, volitional muscular
effort, or whether it is primarily attained through unconscious
neural activation. For example, one method of correcting
posture with anterior pelvic tilt and lumbar lordosis
(swayback) and ribs forward is to use conscious muscular
effort such as engaging the abdominals to bring the pelvis
back to neutral, and pulling the rib cage toward the pelvis.
Another method is to visualize a line of energy that starts at
the center of the body and goes simultaneously down into
the earth and upward past the top of the head, and imagine
this line of energy elongating the body in the vertical axis.
Changes to alignment using this method would occur due to
altered neural recruitment patterns on an unconscious level.
Hypothetically, it would seem that changes made in quiet
stance through conscious muscular effort would have less
success in transferring to locomotor conditions than changes
made at the unconscious level of neuromuscular patterning.
This hypothesis has been at the core of the theories known
as the body therapies, or somatic practices. [1,2,19,56,57]
DYNAMIC ALIGNMENT AS ONGOING NEUROMUSCULAR RESPONSES
The Somatic Practices
In the early 1920s, Todd challenged the accepted notions of
how to improve posture. Her philosophy was to use visualization,
rather than to direct muscles voluntarily to change posture.
In her 1931 article “Our Strains and Tensions” in Progressive
Education Magazine, Todd called her approach the
“opposite of fixity” (as cited by Matt41), and suggested that
awareness and mental processes could alter alignment by affecting
unconscious neuromuscular activity. In her view, establishing
new alignment patterns requires two facilities: (1) the facility
to form adequate mental concepts for stimulating activity in
deep-lying muscles, and (2) the kinesthetic sense that gives rise
to the perception of movement, position, and strain. [57]
Sweigard [56] developed her early definitions of good alignment
from the work of Sir Arthur Keith, [30] who delineated
traditional standards related to the placement of various
parts of the body. Following in the ideology and writings of
Keith and Todd, Sweigard was fully aware of the neuromuscular
component of alignment as well. Her system, Ideokinesis,
used imagery and kinesthetic awareness to affect the relationship
and alignment of skeletal parts, by changing
habitual neuromuscular patterns. These changes should not,
in her view, be complicated by any voluntary holding or positioning
of body parts. Matt [41] discusses Sweigard’s work, stating
that voluntary holding patterns succeed only in producing
momentary improvement. Lasting change is more likely
to occur with kinesthetic re-education, designed to improve
underlying, unconscious neuromuscular habits. In
Sweigard’s 1929–1931 study of the results of Ideokinesis,56
she used a measurement device called the Posturimeter, a
large box with rods at various levels to determine vertical and
horizontal positions of various skeletal parts. The subjects
did 15 weekly Ideokinesis sessions, and multiple measurements
were taken using the Posturimeter. Although there was
no control group in her study, Sweigard observed positive
changes in many segmental relationships.
Bell [4] describes the Alexander technique as psychophysical
education that enables a person to become aware of tensions
in the body. She defines inhibition as a function of the nervous
system that eliminates unwanted activity. Inhibition
occurs at the unconscious or reflex level and helps coordinate
movement. In this view, the unconscious postural mechanisms
underlie the focal, or volitional actions. Similarly,
Lessinger37 is a Feldenkrais practitioner who states, “Improvement
of alignment is best achieved through the kinesthetic
sense rather than through visual assessment and mechanical
forcing of corrections” (p. 329). The somatic practices share
this emphasis on awareness as the process, and on altering
unconscious neuromuscular patterns to improve both static
and dynamic alignment as the goal.
Although there is extensive anecdotal and experiential evidence
of the value of the somatic practices in altering alignment
at the unconscious neuromuscular level, there are few
controlled dance studies that have been conducted in this
area, and the results are mixed. [18,23,33,49,55,56] However, the
research in motor learning and motor control may lend evidence
supporting the theories and practices of these long
respected somatic practitioners and dance educators. The
motor control literature shares the view that posture is a
dynamic process of ongoing neuromuscular adaptations on
an unconscious level of recruitment.
Early Studies in Motor Learning and Motor Control
In the field of motor control and motor learning, there have
been numerous controlled studies examining postural reflexes,
that is, the unconscious neuromuscular patterns that underlie
or accompany voluntary action, dating back to the 1960s and
continuing to more current research. [3,6,7,10,13,14,16,17,21,35,39,40,45,63]
In the motor control literature, posture is described as the
control of the body’s position in space in relation to two
aspects: (1) stability, which is the ability to control the center
of weight relative to the base of support, and (2) orientation,
which is the ability to control the relationship of body parts
to each other and to the environment. Studies examining
unconscious muscle responses to loss of balance used a
device called the perturbation platform, which could shift
forward or backward, or tilt upward or downward. [45] Subjects
were not warned as to when the platform would move or
what kind of perturbation would occur, and thus, they could
not anticipate the timing or the nature of the perturbation.
Two types of reactions to loss of balance were already understood
prior to these studies. One is called the stretch reflex
(or more formally, the myotatic reflex) and refers to the rapid
contraction of a muscle in response to quick or extreme
stretch. Onset latencies measure the time between the onset
of the disturbance to balance, and the onset of the reacting
muscles. The onset latencies for stretch reflex are less than 50
milliseconds. For example, when the ankle is suddenly
twisted stepping off a curb, certain stretch-sensitive neurons
in the stretched ankle muscles send signals that loop from
the ankle muscles to the spinal cord, directly onto neurons
that tell that stretched muscle to contract to protect itself.
Because this sequence does not have to involve pathways all
the way to the brain and back, the response time is extremely
rapid. It is called a “short-loop” response.
A second response to loss of balance is volitional choice or
reaction, in situations where there is time to evaluate the
problem and respond accordingly. These response times are
slow, relative to stretch reflex, and are called “long-loop”
responses. The perturbation platform studies demonstrated a
third response to disturbances to balance. Key points are: (1)
Both the timing and the sequencing of muscle activation in
response to the unexpected perturbations were highly constant
across normal subjects and multiple trials. (2) The
timing of onset latencies of muscle activation was too rapid to
be long-loop or volitional response to loss of balance, but was
too slow to be short-loop or stretch reflex response. Hence, a
third motor program or neuromuscular synergy not previously
defined was discovered. These responses were termed automatic
postural synergies or unconscious postural reflexes.
For example, when the perturbation platform moved forward,
the body would begin to fall backward. The automatic
muscle activation sequence to this perturbation was tibialis
anterior, followed by quadriceps, followed by abdominals. [45]
When the perturbation platform moved backward, the body
would begin to fall forward, and the unconscious muscle activation
sequence was gastrocnemius, followed by hamstrings,
followed by trunk extensors. The activation in these cases was
distal to proximal (starting at the ankle and moving upward
to the trunk), and thus, this strategy was named the ankle
strategy. Because these responses occur after the disturbance,
they are also referred to as reactive or compensatory responses.
In a later study, Horak and Nashner [26] found that when
they altered the size of the support base, a proximal to distal
activation was observed. The support base was a 2 4-inch
piece of wood, and the subjects did not have their heels
placed on the support. With the heels unable to support the
weight, adjustment to balance moved up to the hip joint.
Thus, this strategy was called the hip strategy. These studies
suggest that the body can alter its strategy to accommodate
various conditions. This potential for variation of a muscle
synergy is referred to as plasticity. In other words, rather than
being hard-wired, synergies show differing organizations in
response to varying support surfaces and recent experiences.
Researchers have also been interested in what is occurring
when the disturbance to balance was due to a planned action,
rather than an unexpected perturbation. A study by Cordo
and Nashner13 involving pushing and pulling on a fixed
handle demonstrated that there are proactive or anticipatory
responses in the legs and trunk that occur prior to the
planned, voluntary arm movements. These responses are similar
to the reactive or compensatory responses observed on
the perturbation platform. For example, with the push on
the handle, which would cause the body to fall backward, the
unconscious, anticipatory pattern shown was tibials ->
quadriceps -> abdominals (distal to proximal), used to
counter the anticipated force of the hand movement and its
disturbances to stability. Once the hand was supported
against anticipated perturbation to equilibrium, the volitional
triceps brachii activated. With the pull on the handle,
which would cause the body to fall forward, the unconscious, anticipatory pattern was gastrocnemius -> hamstrings ->
trunk extensors, and then the volitional biceps brachii activated.
It is interesting to note that when the task was repeated
leaning on a chest support, no postural muscles in the lower
limbs and trunk activated during the push or pull. Thus,
studies suggest three key principles governing postural
responses: (1) With prior knowledge of the event, postural
responses will anticipate disturbances that can cause loss of
balance, termed feedforward or anticipatory responses. (2)
When surprises occur, such as the body’s falling forward as
the bus suddenly stops, feedback mechanisms elicit fast corrective
measures, or compensatory responses. (3) When the
body is supported by an outside stabilizing structure, normal
postural responses that occur when the feet are the sole supports
for stability are altered. Given the view in motor control
research that posture is a dynamic process of ongoing
neuromuscular adaptations on an unconscious level of
recruitment, how do these unconscious responses coordinate
with voluntary action?
Integration between Postural Responses and Voluntary Movement
Studies have indicated that the focal (voluntary) task and
postural components of a voluntary movement need to be
coordinated.35 The term integrative control refers to the integration
of unconscious postural mechanisms into ongoing
voluntary movement, including locomotor mechanisms. For
example, if a dancer were running across the stage, and suddenly
slipped and lost balance, the postural reflexes to maintain
balance would need to be integrated into the running
pattern so that the dancer could continue locomoting across
the stage. Frank and Earl22 state “Postural adjustments that
accompany movement serve to prevent or minimize the displacement
of the center of gravity and thereby allow safe and
efficient performance of movement” (p. 103). Hence, compensatory
adjustments to unexpected perturbation occur
during locomotion as well as during stance.
A study by Forssberg [21] demonstrated that anticipatory postural
reflexes precede voluntary (or focal) movements during
locomotion in a similar way to anticipatory responses in
static stance. Subjects walking on a treadmill and pushing or
pulling on a handle demonstrate postural responses in the
legs that are superimposed on the locomotor activity. However,
the type of anticipatory response is phase-dependent;
that is, it varies with each leg depending on which phase of
the walking action is occurring when the arm movement
occurs.46 Similarly, compensatory responses to unexpected
perturbations are phase-dependent during locomotion. This
demonstrates again the variability of the postural reflexes to
conditions.
Additionally, it has been suggested that both postural and
focal components of movement can be directed from higher
centers in the brain, and they are probably organized in parallel
and independent processes. [36] This means that it is not
essential that the postural muscles be recruited prior to the
voluntary movement. The muscles can be influenced by a
variety of factors, and there may be some control exerted by
the subject over the order of recruitment between the postural
and focal activation. Lee et al. [36] found that in a study
involving free arm movement (no fixed handle to push or
pull), the recruitment order of postural and focal muscles was
affected by temporal aspects. When the movement was selfpaced,
the postural muscles were recruited prior to the arm
muscles. However, for fast-paced movements triggered by a
visual cue, the postural and focal muscles fired at the same
time. This again supports the idea of variability in the postural
mechanisms, and suggests that behavioral and mechanical
factors can influence the strategies used to maintain balance
during voluntary movement. This plasticity of the
postural reflexes raises an important question: Can postural
reflexes be learned or modified, and if so, how can training
enhance these unconscious neuromuscular responses?
Postural Reflexes and Training
There are numerous studies in the motor control literature
suggesting that trained subjects have different postural
reflexes than untrained subjects. [8,9,15,39,43,44,47,53] However, it
should be noted before examining these studies that while
they compare trained and untrained subjects, they are not
training studies. Thus, the question of “nature versus nurture”
remains unanswered. Are these trained subjects different
from the controls because of their specialized training, or
does elite activity select for people with inherently superior
neuromuscular response mechanisms? Future research could
indicate how training might influence or affect automatic
responses of the body, that is, how learning is achieved at this
level of neural activity. At this point in time, there are no
training studies clearly confirming that training is the primary
reason for superior postural responses in highly skilled
subjects. However, there are studies examining effects of
training on the ability of older adults to balance. [27,28,64] In particular,
standing balance training in elderly subjects resulted
in significant reduction in body sway under certain conditions,
reduced co-activation of antagonist muscles, and reduction
of onset latencies of specific muscles during disturbances
to balance. Further, effects transferred to other
balance tests, and remained improved for at least one month
after training. While these studies do not involve elite
dancers or athletes, they are certainly encouraging. It is,
therefore, of value to examine the literature and the studies
comparing trained and untrained subjects, and to explore
potential training effects in elite individuals.
Frank and Earl [22] state that postural and focal sets (programming
for muscle synergies) lead to the selection of specific
motor programs, and this occurs in various areas of the
central nervous system, including the motor cortex, the
brainstem motor regions, and the spinal cord. Feedback from
the environment is provided by somatosensory neurons in
the proprioceptive, visual, and vestibular systems, and this
feedback is compared with the internal model to determine
success of the task. The discrepancy between what is expected
and what is actual is used to modify the central nervous
system model. This last step or phase contributes to the learning
of novel movement tasks.
Meglin and Woollacott [42] discuss the association of postural
adjustments (called postural presets), and state that this
is made at a high neural level, in the association cortex, the
basal ganglia, and the neocerebellum, where movement
preparation and initiation originate. However, the link
between movement and posture occurs at a lower level, the
medulla and spinal cord. It is suggested that neural modification
underlying balance improvement occurs at this lower
level of sensorimotor reflex loops. [15] The question still arises
as to what types of training might enhance the temporal,
scalar (amount of effort), and spatial connections between
unconscious and voluntary mechanisms.
Chatfield and Barr [11] considered three principles of training:
anticipatory attention, central muscular activation, and
simultaneous synergistic organization. They hypothesize that
training utilizing these three principles leads to temporal
improvements within and between unconscious postural synergies
and intentional voluntary (conscious) synergies. This
view of training is consistent with the somatic and the motor
control perspectives that posture is a dynamic process of
ongoing neuromuscular adaptations on an unconscious level
of recruitment.
Several studies in the literature compare the balance skills
of trained athletes and untrained subjects. Generally these
studies indicate superior postural responses by trained subjects,
and the researchers suggest that postural reflexes can be
adjusted to higher performance by training. [8,9,15,29,39,47] Trained
subjects showed more stability in one-legged stance8 and
demonstrated anticipatory, as opposed to compensatory,
responses to planned balance tasks.39 Brandt and Paulus9
questioned what kind of training might produce improved
balance, and concluded that subjects should be introduced
to increasingly unstable conditions in order to enhance balance
skills.
Debu and colleagues15 looked at several studies including
elite dancers and athletes in wrestling, baseball, football, water
skiing, gymnastics, and basketball. In each case, they found differences
between trained and untrained subjects, with trained
subjects showing superior balance skills. In a study involving
dancers, Shick et al. [53] found cross-sectional differences between
levels of dancers, with advanced dancers having superior balance
skills to intermediate or beginning dancers.
It has further been suggested that trained subjects develop
new postural synergies. Pedotti and colleagues [47] tested postural
synergies during axial movements, specifically fast backward
trunk movements, and compared gymnasts with
untrained subjects. This movement is fairly familiar in the
training of gymnasts, but uncommon for untrained subjects.
The untrained had a synchronous response, that is, the focal
trunk movements and the unconscious postural reflexes in
the lower limbs occurred simultaneously. The gymnasts, on
the other hand, had sequential responses, with anticipatory
activity in the lower limbs occurring prior to the fast backward
trunk movement. Even though they did not test for
training effects, these researchers suggest that long-term
learning of new postural strategies may be caused by training.
Mouchnino et al. [44] compared dancers with nondancers in
the performance of a well-known dance movement task,
dégagé à la seconde to 45 degrees from turned out first position.
They defined two phases, the ballistic phase, in which
the gesture leg initiates the dégagé, and the adjustment
phase, in which the body is finding a new balance on the support
leg. They observed the following four differences: (1)
The dancers reached the final position (steady state) at the
end of the ballistic phase, having a very short adjustment
phase. Non-dancers had two distinct phases, including a long
adjustment phase. (2) The dancers had feedforward control
(anticipatory postural reflexes) to the dégagé; that is, the muscles
of the supporting leg and trunk fired prior to the dégagé
gesture, whereas the nondancers had compensatory
responses. (3) The dancers minimized the center of gravity
displacement to the support leg, whereas the nondancers
showed a larger displacement of the center of gravity. (4) The
subjects demonstrated two distinct movement strategies,
called the inclination strategy (nondancers) and the translation
strategy (dancers). In the inclination strategy, the hips
and head tilted to accommodate the shift of weight, and in
the translation strategy, the hips remained level and the
entire structure shifted over to the new support. Like Pedotti
et al., [47] even without actual pre-/posttraining data, Mouchnino
et al. [44] suggest that these differences may be the results
of training.
Monasterio and colleagues [43] conducted a follow-up study
comparing dancers and nondancers performing a similar
dance movement task, développé à la seconde to 45 degrees
from cou-de-pied devant. Results showed trends suggesting
that the dancers had anticipatory postural responses, while
the nondancers had compensatory responses. While trials
included both fast-paced and self-paced trials, it was the fastpaced
trials that demonstrated the anticipatory responses in
the dancers. Further, they also differed in using the translation
versus the inclination strategy. However, as with the preceding
studies, they are all simply cross-sectional snapshots of
group differences at one point in time as opposed to training
data collected repeatedly on the same sample as they progress
through training. Therefore, it is unknown at this time
whether the differences are due to innate predisposition, or
to the training.
Dance investigators have also been interested in examining
other aspects of neuromotor mechanisms in trained
versus untrained subjects, including skeletal alignment in
dynamic situations, and neuromuscular patterns specific to
high level dance skills. Ryman and Ranney [52] studied two variations
of grand battement devant, parallel and turned out,
and compared four elite ballet dancers. Using electromyography
(EMG), they found that each dancer had a unique pattern
of muscle activation, even though the subjects were
matched in terms of training and body type, and they all
visually achieved the same goal. Through biomechanical
analysis, the authors also discovered that executing the movement
at the barre is not the same as in the center of the room
without a support structure. Without the support of the
barre, the dancer needed to adjust the center of weight backward
in space so that it was over the one supporting leg. At
the barre, no adjustment was made. The authors suggest that
training at the barre, while valuable for developing strength and various technical and artistic skills, does not yield an
improvement in equilibrium support mechanisms.
A recent study by Wilmerding and colleagues61 supports
their conclusion. Nineteen professional ballet dancers (aged
18–45 years) executed développé devant at the barre and in
the center. Using EMG analysis, it was discovered that the
muscle activations of the gesture leg (the focal activity) were
the same in the two situations. However, the muscle activations
of the supporting leg (the postural activity) were different.
Activity of the tibialis anterior and abductor hallicus
muscles (unconscious, postural activation) was significantly
greater in the center than at the barre, suggesting that using
an external support mechanism is not the same as working
unsupported to prepare the body for equilibrium challenges.
Chatfield and colleagues12 found that even when nondancers
were given intensive training in performance of a
select voluntary movement strategy involving a well-rehearsed,
whole-body action, the dancers were significantly better than
the nondancer controls. Thus, the difference between the two
groups was not simply a testing effect, in which the dancers
were familiar with the task prior to the study, but the nondancers
were not. This suggests that long-term training may
produce different results than short-term training, although
the differences might still be the result of the selection process
in dancers advancing through the ranks.
Two recent studies [58,59] also provide evidence that dance
training might cause changes to neuromuscular patterning.
In doing EMG measurements on stance, demi plié and grand
plié, these studies found that ballet and modern dancers
showed differences in certain strategies, and furthermore,
that there was high individual variability, both within and
across subjects. However, Trepman et al. [58] also suggest that
these results may relate to body structure differences, rather
than training. Because there is a selection process in career
paths in dance, there is no way to know, without a longitudinal
training study with matched subjects, whether it is the
training causing the differences, or the initial body types and
neuromotor systems of genetically gifted dancers.
Recent dance researchers have examined posture as a
dynamic process of ongoing neuromuscular adaptations on
an unconscious level of recruitment. In a study conducted by
Krasnow et al., [33] the alignment of dancers was measured using
light-reflective markers and a motion analysis system. The
light-reflective markers were placed on the bony landmarks
consistent with the plumb line view of static vertical alignment.
Both static alignment and the return to vertical alignment
following an off-center, dynamic torso movement were
investigated. Following pretests, four groups were involved in
training: imagery only, conditioning only, conditioning with
imagery, and controls who received no training. While there
were no significant differences between groups in static
stance, one group, the conditioning with imagery group, was
significantly better at achieving vertical skeletal alignment
immediately following the torso movement. This study suggests
that training might influence alignment in dynamic situations,
even when static alignment has not been altered.
Gamboian et al. [23] also conducted alignment studies during
dynamic movement, and performed repeated trials over several
weeks. They found that alignment is not only variable by
condition but also variable day to day. Further, lumbar lordosis
and pelvic tilt were independent of each other in this
study, and one could change, while the other did not. This
variability across a variety of factors suggests that research on
dancers and alignment needs to incorporate a methodology
covering multiple testing sessions and multiple trials, as well
as providing an in-depth look at individual strategies.
Finally, the recent text on spinal stabilization by Richardson
and colleagues51 examines cutting-edge research on the
unconscious activation of the transversus abdominis in perturbations
to balance. The studies cited in this text were conducted
by research groups headed by Cresswell and Richardson
in the 1990s. Until recently, it was extremely difficult to
measure muscle activation in deep lying muscles, as surface
electrodes are not useful in measuring their function. In the
late 1980s, ultrasound-guided techniques for needle insertion
were developed, making it feasible to monitor the activation
of the transversus abdominis in a variety of conditions,
including static and dynamic trials of spine flexion and
extension. Unlike the other abdominal muscles (rectus abdominis,
obliquus externus abdominis, and obliquus internus
abdominis), the transversus abdominus is not a spine
mover—that is, it does not contribute to flexion, extension, or
rotation of the spine; rather, it is active in breathing and in
stabilization. The transversus abdominis was active throughout
the trials in both flexion and extension, indicating that
this muscle acts independently of the other abdominal muscles,
which do not participate during extension. They also
noted that in normal subjects, the transversus abdominis
consistently demonstrated anticipatory activity to disturbances
to balance, such as movement of limbs or trunk loading
with weight. However, subjects with chronic low back
pain and/or injury did not have consistent transverse abdominal
responses. The responses in these subjects were late,
erratic, or nonexistent. The researchers suggest that these differences
in impaired subjects are related to motor control,
that is, recruitment of the transverse abdominal muscle, and
are not an issue of muscular strength or endurance. These
authors are currently working on methods of training to
enhance transversus abdominis activity in subjects demonstrating
chronic low back pain and/or injury, and these
methods may be useful for dance training as well.
SUGGESTIONS FOR TRAINING ENHANCEMENT AND RESEARCH
Both the somatic literature and the motor control literature
have suggested that posture is a dynamic process of ongoing
neuromuscular adaptations on an unconscious level of
recruitment, and that training can alter or modify these
unconscious neuromotor patterns. While the studies comparing
trained and untrained subjects cannot confirm that training
improves postural reflexes and neuromotor responses,
they certainly provide evidence that better dancers have different
postural responses than beginners or nondancers.
Therefore, suggestions for training that specifically address
these issues can be made. These suggestions are applicable to both the training requisite for developing elite professional
dancers and the training that improves an individual involved
in dance who will not reach the top levels of performance.
Several ideas for enhancing dance training are suggested by
the writings in somatic practices, dance, and motor control.
First, both cognitive awareness and kinesthetic awareness may
well be keys to improving the automatic neuromuscular patterns
governing alignment and balancing skills. Dancers
should be encouraged to understand how the body functions,
and how this relates to personal anatomical possibilities and
limitations. The somatic practices can provide an exceptional
method for developing discerning perceptions of the body,
and allowing exploration in a conducive atmosphere.
Second, movement experiences may be crucial to the
development of alignment in dynamic contexts as well as to
the improvement of postural reflexes. Working on static vertical
alignment may provide an experience of neutral “home
base,” but correcting skeletal placement in stance, making
conscious and voluntary changes in the stacking of body segments,
does not necessarily provide for the best anticipatory
responses to dynamic movement through space. Both situations
might need attention. Images and processes that
dancers can apply while moving may be useful to this type of
learning. The issue that is central to this discussion is transfer.
How can the dancer transfer what is learned and
absorbed at an unconscious level during static alignment to
dynamic situations, involving speed, direction change, and
perturbations to balance? It is possible that heightened kinesthetic
awareness and the use of imagery and visualization
techniques may be keys to transfer of training, as suggested by
somatic practitioners.
Third, it is crucial that dancers practice balancing and
movement tasks in situations where they are not supported
by external structures, such as sitting and lying on the floor
and standing at the barre. While these training mechanisms
provide valuable skills and lessons, they do not challenge the
postural reflexes in the same way as center and traveling
work. Again, the issue of transfer is crucial in the training setting.
Skills learned at the barre or sitting and lying on a support
surface may or may not be applicable to similar skills
required in unsupported standing and locomotor movement.
Fourth, it is suggested that movement perturbing balance
at fast pace provides challenges to the neuromotor system
that may not be occurring in slower-paced experiences. A full
range of tempos and dynamics might provide the best possible
training for equilibrium responses, thereby employing the
plasticity of the neuromuscular system. Contextual variety
may be a key to neuromotor excellence. Dancers should be
given progressively more unstable and challenging conditions
to balance. In this way, the unconscious neuromuscular patterns
can receive the greatest range of experiences to enhance
new neuromotor strategies.
The studies in dance science and in motor control have
raised as many questions for researchers as for educators.
Training studies are needed to determine what can be modified
on the neural level, in terms of improving both static vertical
alignment and dynamic neuromuscular responses.
Issues of transfer from static to dynamic alignment, and from
one context to another, are not fully understood at this time.
Does slow practice transfer to fast-paced movement? Does
work at the barre or lying and sitting on the floor transfer to
center standing work and traveling work? How can
researchers effectively measure alignment in static and
dynamic situations, and how variable is alignment across
days, across conditions, and across subjects? If alignment is as
individual as the existing research is suggesting, a shift from
group design to single-subject design in research methodology
may be necessary. Collapsing data to group means may be
masking important individualized strategies that would
reveal valuable information about both static and dynamic
alignment strategies.
CONCLUSION
This article has attempted to review the literature regarding
various approaches to alignment, and to present a view of
alignment that is one of dynamic neuromuscular responses.
This perspective incorporates the ideas concerning skeletal
placement as well as those exploring the automatic postural
reflexes, and their integration with voluntary, focal neuromuscular
tasks. Future research will hopefully provide evidence
of the interaction of skeletal placement, postural
reflexes, and locomotion, as well as other neuromuscular
issues, and the potential effects of training on the neuromotor
system. In 1950, Wells wrote the first edition of Kinesiology:
The Scientific Basis of Human Motion, and addressed the
complex issue of posture. In the 9th edition of this text,
Luttgens and Hamilton38 stated:
By this time the reader should understand that posture influences
all we do and that it is not a static but a dynamic configuration. It
should also be understood that no single ideal postural model is
appropriate for all individuals. Instead, there must be an understanding
of the principles that govern efficient posture. These
principles must then be applied to each individual (p. 453).
This statement is followed by a list of 13 principles that
include the following as influencing or defining posture: the
line of gravity and its relationship to the weight bearing segments,
the relative extension of weight-bearing joints, energy
expenditure, mechanical function of joints, use of muscle
force (including the development of antigravity muscles and
the balance between antagonist muscle groups), flexibility,
coordination (including neuromuscular control and postural
reflexes), kinesthetic awareness, organic function, personality,
emotional states, age, and aesthetics. The final principle
states, “In the last analysis, both the static and dynamic posture
of any individual should be judged on the basis of how
well it meets the demands made upon it throughout a lifetime”
(p. 455). This broad definition of posture, encompassing
the physical, neurological, emotional, and psychological
aspects of the whole person, also raises the issue of lifelong
endurance and function. Are researchers asking questions
about alignment that concern only optimal performance
during the short years of a dancer’s career, or can the perspective
include a concern for the years after the dancing has
ended? Are educators teaching principles of alignment that
satisfy a given aesthetic, regardless of its long-term impact on the body, or is the health and well-being of the dancer at the
forefront of pedagogical developments? It is hoped that
researchers and educators can begin to develop approaches to
alignment that are flexible in relation to respecting individual
differences, and inclusive of a variety of aspects of human
function during one’s entire life.
ABOUT AUTHORS
Ms. Krasnow is at York University, Toronto, Ontario, Canada; Ms.
Monasterio is at Carpe Diem Wellness Center, Eugene, Oregon; and Dr.
Chatfield is at the University of Oregon, Eugene, Oregon.
BIBLIOGRAPHY
1. Alexander FM: The Use of the Self. Long Beach, CA, Centerline Press,
1985.
2. Bartenieff I: Body Movement: Coping with the Environment. New
York, Gordon and Breach, Science Publishers, 1980.
3. Belen’kii V, Gurfinkel V, Pal’tsev Y: Elements of control of voluntary
movements. Biophysics, 12: 154–161, 1967.
4. Bell JL: The Alexander technique for dancers. In Fitt SS (ed.): Dance
Kinesiology (2nd ed.). New York, Schirmer Books, 1996, pp. 331–335.
5. Blasis C: An Elementary Treatise upon the Theory and Practice of the
Art of Dancing. Translated by M. S. Evans. New York, Kamin Dance
Publishers, 1953.
6. Bouisset S., Zattara M: A sequence of postural movements precedes
voluntary movement. Neurosci Lett 22:263–270, 1981.
7. Bouisset S, Zattara M: Biomechanical study of the programming of
anticipatory postural adjustments associated with voluntary movement.
J Biomech 20/8:735–742, 1987.
8. Brandt T, Buchele W, Krafczyk S: Training effects on experimental postural
instability: A model for clinical therapy. In Bles W, Brandt T
(eds.): Disorders of Posture and Gait. Amsterdam: Elsevier Science
Publishers, 1986, pp. 353–356.
9. Brandt T, Paulus W: Postural retraining in exceptional populations. In
Woollacott MH, Shumway-Cook A (eds.): Development of Posture and
Gait: Across the Lifespan. Columbia, SC, University of South Carolina
Press, 1989, pp. 299–319.
10. Brown J, Frank J: Influence of event anticipation on postural actions
accompanying voluntary movement. Exp Brain Res 67:645–650, 1987.
11. Chatfield SJ, Barr S: Towards a testable hypothesis of training principles
for the neuromuscular facilitation of human movement. Dance
Res J 26/1:8–14, 1994.
12. Chatfield S J, Barr S, Sveistrup H, Woollacott MH: Electromyographic
and kinematic analysis of movement repatterning in dance. Impulse
Int J Dance Sci Med Educ 4/3:220–234, 1996.
13. Cordo P, Nashner L: Properties of postural adjustments associated
with rapid arm movements. J Neurophysiol 47:287–302, 1982.
14. Crenna P, Frigo C, Massion J, Pedotti A: Forward and backward axial
synergies in man. Exp Brain Res 65:538–548, 1987.
15. Debu B, Werner L, Woollacott M: In Woollacott MH, Shumway-Cook
A (eds.): Development of Posture and Gait: Across the Lifespan.
Columbia, SC, University of South Carolina Press, 1989, pp. 280–298
16. Debu B, Woollacott M: Effects of gymnastics training on postural
responses to stance perturbations. J Mot Behav 20:273–300, 1988.
17. Duffosse M, Hugon M, Massion J, Paulignan Y: Two modes of adaptive
change to perturbations of forearm posture. In Posture and Gait:
Development, Adaptation, and Modulation. Amsterdam, Elsevier Science
Publishers, 1988, pp. 217–225.
18. Fairweather MM, Sidaway B: Ideokinetic imagery as a postural development
technique. Res Q Exerc Sport 64/4:385–392, 1993.
19. Feldenkrais M: Awareness through Movement. New York, Harper and
Row, 1972.
20. Fitt SS: Dance Kinesiology (2nd ed.). New York, Schirmer Books, 1996.
21. Forssberg H: Anticipatory postural adjustments during human locomotion.
The London Symposium 39:72–76, 1987.
22. Frank J, Earl M: Coordination of posture & movement. In Movement
Science: An American Physical Therapy Association Monograph, 108.
Alexandria, VA, American Physical Therapy Association, 1990.
23. Gamboian N, Chatfield SJ, Woollacott MH, Klug GA: Effect of dance
technique training and somatic training on pelvic tilt and lumbar lordosis
alignment during quiet stance and dynamic dance movement. J
Dance Med Sci 3/1:5–14, 1999.
24. Hanna T: Bodies in Revolt: A Primer in Somatic Thinking. New York,
Holt, 1970.
25. Hellebrandt FA, Kubin D, Longfield WM, Kelso, LEA: The base of support
in stance: Its characteristics and the influence of shoes upon the
location of the center of weight. Physiother Rev 17/6:231–234, 1937.
26. Horak F, Nashner L: Central programming of postural movements:
Adaptation to altered support-surface configurations. J Neurophysiol
55:1369–1381, 1986.
27. Hu MH, Woollacott MH: Multisensory training of standing balance in
older adults: I. Postural stability and one-leg stance balance. J Gerontol
49/2:52–M61, 1994.
28. Hu MH, Woollacott MH: Multisensory training of standing balance in
older adults: II. Kinematic and electromyographic postural responses.
J Gerontol 49/2:M62–M71, 1994.
29. Kamon E, Gormley J: Muscular activity pattern for skilled performance
and during learning of a horizontal bar exercise. Ergonomics
11/4:345–357, 1968.
30. Keith A: The Engines of the Human Body: Being the Substance of
Christmas Lectures Given at the Royal Institution of Great Britain,
Christmas 1916–1917. Philadelphia, J. B. Lippincott Company, 1933.
31. Kerr G, Krasnow D, Mainwaring L: The nature of dance injuries. Med
Probl Perform Art 7:25–29, 1992.
32. Kirstein L, Stuart M: The Classic Ballet: Basic Technique and Terminology.
New York, Alfred A. Knopf, 1976 (first published 1952).
33. Krasnow DH, Chatfield SJ, Barr S, Jensen JL, Dufek JS: Imagery and
conditioning practices for dancers. Dance Res J 29/1:43–64, 1997.
34. Lawson J: Classic ballet: Common Faults in Young Dancers and Their
Training. New York, Theatre Arts Books, 1973.
35. Lee WA: Anticipatory control of postural and task muscles during
rapid arm flexion. J Mot Behav 12:185–196, 1980.
36. Lee WA, Buchanan T, Rogers M: Effects of arm acceleration and
behavioral conditions on the organization of postural adjustments
during arm flexion. Exp Brain Res 66:257–270, 1987.
37. Lessinger C: The nature of the Feldenkrais method and its value to
dancers. In Fitt SS (ed.): Dance Kinesiology (2nd ed.). New York,
Schirmer Books, 1996, pp. 325–331.
38. Luttgens K, Hamilton N: The standing posture. In Kinesiology: Scientific
Basis of Human Motion (9th ed.). Dubuque, IA, Brown & Benchmark,
1997, pp. 445–459.
39. Massion J: Movement, posture, and equilibrium: Interaction and coordination.
Prog Neurobiol 38:35–56, 1992.
40. Massion J, Duffosse M: Coordination between posture and movement:
Why and how? News Physiol 3(June):88–93, 1988.
41. Matt P: The nature of Ideokinesis and its value for dancers. In Fitt SS
(ed.): Dance Kinesiology (2nd ed.). New York, Schirmer Books, 1996,
pp. 335–341.
42. Meglin J, Woollacott M: The neural choreography underlying a pirouette-
arabesque. Kinesiol Med Dance 14:95–105, 1992.
43. Monasterio RA, Chatfield SJ, Jensen JL, Barr S: Postural adjustment
for voluntary leg movement in dancers. Unpublished master’s thesis,
University of Oregon, 1994.
44. Mouchnino L, Aurenty R, Massion J, Pedotti A: Coordination between
equilibrium and head–trunk orientation during leg movement: A new
strategy built up by training. J Neurophysiol 67/6:1587–1598, 1992.
45. Nashner LM: Fixed patterns of rapid postural responses among muscles
during stance. Exp Brain Res 30:13–24, 1977.
46. Nashner LM, Forssberg H: The phase dependent organization of postural
adjustments associated with arm pull movements while walking.
J Neurophysiol 55:1382–1394, 1986.
47. Pedotti A, Crenna P, Deat A, Frigo C, Massion J: Postural synergies in
axial movements: Short and long-term adaptation. Expe Brain Res 74:
3–8, 1989.
48. Plastino JG: Physical screening of the dancer: General methodologies
and procedures. In Solomon R, Minton S, Solomon J (eds.): Preventing
Dance Injuries: An Interdisciplinary Perspective. Reston, VA,
American Alliance for Health, Physical Education, Recreation, and
Dance, 1990, pp. 155–175.
49. Pokora SL: An investigation of imaging as a facilitator for the development
of the dancer. Masters Abstracts International, 27/03, 318. (University
Microfilms International No. 13-35842), 1988.
50. Ralph R: The life and works of John Weaver. London, Dance Books,
Ltd., 1985.
51. Richardson C, Jull G, Hodges P, Hides J: Therapeutic Exercise for
Spinal Segmental Stabilization in Low Back Pain: Scientific Basis and
Clinical Approach. Edinburgh, Churchill Livingstone, 1999.
52. Ryman R, Ranney D: A preliminary investigation of two variations of
the grand battement devant. Dance Res J 11:2–11, 1978/1979.
53. Schick J, Stoner L, Jette N: Relationship between modern-dance experience
and balancing performance. Res Q Exerc Sport 54:79–82, 1983.
54. Schmidt R: Motor Control and Learning: Behavior Emphasis. Champaign,
IL, Human Kinetics Publishers, 1982.
55. Studd KA: Ideokinesis, mental rehearsal and relaxation applied to
dance technique (master’s thesis, University of Oregon, 1983). (Microform
Publications: University of Oregon No. UO-85 61–UO -85 62),
1983.
56. Sweigard LE: Human Movement Potential: Its Ideokinetic Facilitation.
New York, Harper and Row, 1974.
57. Todd ME: 1937. The Thinking Body: A Study of the Balancing Forces
of Dynamic Man. New York, Paul B. Hoeber, Medical Book Department
of Harper & Brothers.
58. Trepman E, Gellman RE, Micheli LJ, De Luca CJ: Electromyographic
analysis of grand-plié in ballet and modern dancers. Med Sci Sports
Exerc 30/12:1708–1720, 1998.
59. Trepman E, Gellman RE, Solomon R, Murthy KR, Micheli LJ, De
Luca CJ: Electromyographic analysis standing posture and demi-plié in
ballet and modern dancers. Med Sci Sports Exerc 26/6:771–782, 1994.
60. Wells K: Kinesiology: The Scientific Basis of Human Motion (4th ed.)
Philadelphia, W. B. Saunders, 1966.
61. Wilmerding V, Heyward V, King M, Fiedler K, Stidley C, Evans B:
Electromyographical Comparison of the Developpe Devant at Barre
and Centre. Paper presented at the conference of the International
Association for Dance Medicine and Science, Tring, England, Oct
1999.
62. Woodhull-McNeal AP, Clarkson PM, James R, Watkin, A, Barrett S:
How linear is dancers’ posture? Med Probl Perform Art 5/4:151–154,
1990.
63. Woollacott M, Bonnet M, Yabe K: Preparatory process for anticipatory
postural adjustment: Modulation of leg muscles reflex pathways
during preparation for arm movements in standing man. Exp Brain
Res 55:253–267, 1984.
64. Woollacott MH, Moore S, Hu MH: Improvements in balance in the
elderly through training in sensory organization abilities. In Stelmach
GE, Hömberg V (eds.): Sensorimotor Impairment in the Elderly.
Netherlands, Kluwer Academic Publishers, 1993, pp. 377–392.